Skip to main content

How listening works

 

If you want to improve your teaching of listening, it’s a good idea to have a sense of what we know about what goes on between the moment that the message made by a human voice hits your ear drum, until the moment in which you form meaning from it. Anderson, writing in 1990, came up with a ‘three phase model’ consisting of ‘perceptual processing’, ‘parsing’ and ‘utilisation’: in layman’s terms meaning that firstly you recognise the sound as familiar language, then you break the sound into words and chunks, and finally you give meaning to them, and / or act on them in some way. Later, Cutler and Clifton split up the ‘parsing’ section into two: firstly ‘segmenting’ – breaking the up the continuous stream of what you hear into individual words – and secondly ‘recognising’, which is the process of using your mental dictionary to look up the words.

Most recently, John Field has been the world expert in second language listening. His book Listening In The Language Classroom (2008) gives a thorough background into second language listening and is highly recommended. Field’s model is the one I used to underpin my PhD work and is the one that almost all researchers refer to when working on listening these days. I will explain it in a little more detail here. All of what’s explained in the next paragraph is taking place in a split second within the mind of the listener.

Once the sound comes in, (Field calls it the ‘speech signal’) the listener instantly starts to use the knowledge they have about how sounds go together in the language – what is permissible and what isn’t. For example, when listening to English, an experienced or native listener knows instinctively that all words only have one emphasised syllable, and will use that information automatically to give them clues about where one word finishes and the next one starts. The next step is to start running to the mental dictionary – to try and identify words based on the sounds that have been heard, and other clues like how common it might be, and the words around it. Then the clues that have been picked up allow the listener to narrow the possibilities of what’s been heard, and grammatical clues like endings for tenses and other little chunks of words come into play to give the listener a better and better sense of what’s going on. Overlaid onto these ‘linguistic’ processes are contextual elements like whether the listener knows what the speaker might have intended, the context of what’s being heard, common sense and general knowledge.

This process isn’t as linear as I set out there, though: it’s more iterative. That means it goes round and round and as a listener you might grasp one sense of what you’ve heard, but then the next bit of input makes you reassess. Consider, for example, saying this to someone in English (it doesn’t work so well if you read it): ‘I stooped to pick a buttercup. What a buttock was doing there, I don’t know.’ If you say it in your mind’s ear, it really illustrates how you change your understanding from buttercup to buttock up after you hear the second sentence – it changes the context, makes you split the ‘buttercup’ sound in two, and forces you to alter your interpretation of ‘pick’ to ‘pick up’.

What this shows, then, is that once some speech hits the listener’s ear, their mind is working ever so hard to form meaning, and is performing extraordinary feats. Every teacher knows that you can ask a child ‘what did I just say?’ and they will parrot it back, but this is no indication that they are actually processing anything that’s being said: that’s the ‘echoic memory’. During listening it is crucial to keep some parts of the input ticking over in the echoic memory while actually processing other parts, and then putting the two together to make meaning. In other words, there’s a whole lot going on – and it’s all in the mind of the listener.

What I’ve set out above is not just about second language – it’s the process that everyone experiences whenever they are decoding a language. The difference when it’s a second language is how automatic each bit of the process is. Because every level that I explained above might be hindered if you are a learner of a second language: does the listener know what sounds exist or not in the new language? How big is their vocabulary and how quickly can they retrieve it from their mental dictionary while more and more words come pouring into the ears? Can they automatically recognise little grammatical cues like tense endings, or markers of gender or plurality? Does their general knowledge or cultural knowledge of the target language extend to being able to apply contextual sense to what they hear? All of these elements are way harder in a second language – even for an advanced listener, and slow down the process. And when the process is slowed down, it’s inevitable that many more hitches might arise. Understanding these hitches is the key to beginning to resolve them and therefore to make better listeners of your students.

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

How listening isn’t reading - and where that might take us

As I said to my daughter ‘I’m going to write a blog about how listening isn’t reading’, she said ‘well, obviously.’ But of course there’s a lot more to it than that. The types of tasks that are set in the language classroom (regardless of target language) might well suggest that listening tasks are identical to reading tasks, with the only difference that the passages are heard by the student rather than read. In my context - teaching French, German and occasionally Spanish to students in an English secondary school, one would be forgiven to assume, therefore, that listening is simply ‘heard reading’. And indeed, the types of passages that my students are exposed to from the exercise books and then in the exams, this is largely the case (a little bit less than it was, perhaps, but still largely the case). The inherent wrongness of this approach is fodder for a different blog post and will no doubt form a thread throughout all the posts on this blog. So rather than dipping into what on ...

Learning to listen and listening to learn

This week I want to dig into the purpose of listening in our language lessons, and where that takes us When I first started teaching, I was using Studio for French and Echo for German (later on I started to teach Spanish too, and worked from Viva). I noticed even in my training year the pattern whereby a set of vocabulary and grammar was the learning outcome for the double page spread. To start with this would be introduced passively - it seemed to alternate between a bit of reading and a bit of listening first, and then the other would come. Then the students would be expected to produce what they had learned.  It struck me as making perfect sense: the students were very clearly listening to learn whatever the language point(s) were for that particular lesson or series of lessons.  So one of the key reasons why we listen, then, is to learn. From a language-learning point of view, that might be listening to learn the language, like the example I have just given above. Or, mor...

The Comprehension Approach

 ‘Now, class, we’re going to do some listening. Everyone quiet’. The teacher shares a task - perhaps a series of questions to answer, perhaps a series of statements to which the student needs to dichotomise (true / false. Positive / negative. Bob / Katie). Then the teacher presses play on their device, and students listen - probably twice - and note down answers.  Finally the teacher recaps. Some might simply give the ‘answers’ (more later on why this is in inverted commas), others might elicit answers from students in various ways. Some might ask the students to note a mark out of five in their books. And then we move on to another task (frequently a speaking exercise in which the students are asked to produce output similar to what they have just heard.) This is a scenario played out in language lessons across the world. It’s what John Field calls ‘The Comprehension Approach’ in his 2008 book Listening in the Language Classroom. (An aside. It’s a brilliant book, rigorous...